المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
المرجع الألكتروني للمعلوماتية

English Language
عدد المواضيع في هذا القسم 6095 موضوعاً
Grammar
Linguistics
Reading Comprehension

Untitled Document
أبحث عن شيء أخر
غزوة الحديبية والهدنة بين النبي وقريش
2024-11-01
بعد الحديبية افتروا على النبي « صلى الله عليه وآله » أنه سحر
2024-11-01
المستغفرون بالاسحار
2024-11-01
المرابطة في انتظار الفرج
2024-11-01
النضوج الجنسي للماشية sexual maturity
2024-11-01
المخرجون من ديارهم في سبيل الله
2024-11-01


Across-the-board effects  
  
858   01:44 صباحاً   date: 12-4-2022
Author : David Odden
Book or Source : Introducing Phonology
Page and Part : 292-9


Read More
Date: 2024-03-05 445
Date: 2023-09-15 473
Date: 2023-05-24 869

Across-the-board effects

Another phenomenon which argues for the autosegmental representation of tone is across-the-board tone change. An illustration of such a tonal effect can be found in Shona. The examples in (21) show that if a noun begins with some number of H tones, those H’s become L when preceded by one of the prefixes né-, sé-, and ché.

As shown in (22) and by the last example of (21), an H tone which is not part of an initial string of H’s will not undergo this lowering process.

The problem is that if we look at a word such as mbúndúdzí as having three H tones, then there is no way to apply the lowering rule to the word and get the right results. Suppose we apply the following rule to a standard segmental representation of this word.

Beginning from /né-mbúndúdzí/, this rule would apply to the first H-toned vowel giving né-mbùndúndzí. However, the rule could not apply again since the vowel of the second syllable is not immediately preceded by the prefix which triggers the rule. And recall from examples such as né-mùrúmé that the rule does not apply to noninitial H tones.

This problem has a simple solution in autosegmental theory, where we are not required to represent a string of n H-toned vowels as having n H tones. Instead, these words can have a single H tone which is associated with a number of vowels.

Given these representations, the tone-lowering process will only operate on a single tone, the initial tone of the noun, but this may be translated into an effect on a number of adjacent vowels.

There is a complication in this rule which gives further support to the autosegmental account of this process. Although this process lowers a string of H tones at the beginning of a noun, when one of these prefixes precedes a prefixed structure, lowering does not affect every initial H tone. When one prefix precedes another prefix which precedes a noun with initial H’s, the second prefix has an L tone and the noun keeps its H tones.

However, if there are three of these prefixes, the second prefix has an L tone, and lowering also affects the first (apparent) string of tones in the noun.

A simple statement like “lower a sequence of adjacent H’s” after an H prefix would be wrong, as these data show. What we see here is an alternating pattern, which follows automatically from the rule that we have posited and the autosegmental theory of representations. Consider the derivation of a form with two prefixes.

The lowering of H on che gives that prefix an L tone, and therefore that prefix cannot then cause lowering of the H’s of the noun. On the other hand, if there are three such prefixes, the first H-toned prefix causes the second prefix to become L, and that prevents prefix 2 from lowering prefix 3. Since prefix 3 keeps its H tone, it therefore can cause lowering of H in the noun.

Thus it is not simply a matter of lowering the tones of any number of vowels. Unlike the traditional segmental theory, the autosegmental model provides a very simple and principled characterization of these patterns of tone lowering.