المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
المرجع الألكتروني للمعلوماتية

English Language
عدد المواضيع في هذا القسم 6142 موضوعاً
Grammar
Linguistics
Reading Comprehension

Untitled Document
أبحث عن شيء أخر المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
ميعاد زراعة الجزر
2024-11-24
أثر التأثير الاسترجاعي على المناخ The Effects of Feedback on Climate
2024-11-24
عمليات الخدمة اللازمة للجزر
2024-11-24
العوامل الجوية المناسبة لزراعة الجزر
2024-11-24
الجزر Carrot (من الزراعة الى الحصاد)
2024-11-24
المناخ في مناطق أخرى
2024-11-24

التكاثف Condensation - التكاثف السطحي – الندى Dew
19-3-2022
أمثلة شاملة عن الاوراق التجاريـة وعملياتـها المحاسبية
6-3-2022
حكم من عجز عن الأصناف الثلاثة في الكفّارة.
18-1-2016
بحر المديد
24-03-2015
الديانات في الصين
26-3-2018
زمن المضاعفة doubling time
10-9-2018

Bristol l  
  
725   08:47 صباحاً   date: 2023-12-16
Author : David Hornsby
Book or Source : Linguistics A complete introduction
Page and Part : 97-5


Read More
Date: 2023-07-31 418
Date: 2023-11-13 793
Date: 19-5-2022 632

Bristol l

Intrusive r occurs in part because loan words ending in unstressed final -a (e.g. India, banana) had no counterparts in English when they entered the language. Speakers therefore assimilated them to something similar, in most cases the final -er lexical set (e.g. reader, carer), for which /r/ is deleted in non-rhotic areas except before a vowel. In Bristol, a rhotic city in the west of England, these words were assimilated not to the -er set but to the -le set (e.g. little, apple), with the result that banana is pronounced ‘bananal’ and pairs such as idea/ideal and area/aerial are homophonous.

 

Because Bristol is so out of step with other English regions in this regard, Bristol l has become widely ridiculed outside the city and is increasingly avoided by all but lowest-status Bristolians as a consequence. An accent feature that is so salient as to attract mockery is called a stereotype.

 

In similar vein, an appeal to underlying representations can illuminate some otherwise puzzling phenomena in Standard French, where many words have an orthographical final e which is not pronounced (‘mute e’):

 

Similarly, many words end in orthographical consonants that are not realized: trop, les, bon. It is not uncommon for pronunciation and spelling to be out of step, but in this case standard French orthography may reflect some important facts about French phonology. In spite of the fact that ‘mute e’ is not pronounced, many phonologists would suggest that vase is underlyingly , and trop underlyingly , with a deletion rule being applied to delete the final segment in each case.

 

Once more, a seemingly counter-intuitive approach captures a number of important generalizations. Firstly, we know that the final mute -e is an orthographic holdover from a time when word-final schwa was pronounced:  was once the normal pronunciation, and remains so in many parts of southern France today. So, rather like our earlier /r/ example from English, our underlying representation captures a synchronic fact about French and a diachronic one. Secondly, even for speakers who don’t normally pronounce the final schwa, there are occasions when this consonant reappears, typically to break up sequences of three consonants, so Jacques est le maître  but Maître Jacques .

 

Similarly, many words have latent word-final consonants which appear before a vowel in what is known as liaison, thus les gens  ‘people’ but les amis ‘friends’ . But why is the final consonant deleted in some cases, but not in others? One answer would be that the final [z] of vase is always pronounced because in its abstract representation it is non-final, and the underlying final schwa ‘blocks’ the deletion rule. The abstraction is arguably therefore a price worth paying for a richer analysis of what French speakers ‘know’ about their language, namely that some word-final consonants are always pronounced, but others are only pronounced in certain environments.