المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
المرجع الألكتروني للمعلوماتية

English Language
عدد المواضيع في هذا القسم 6142 موضوعاً
Grammar
Linguistics
Reading Comprehension

Untitled Document
أبحث عن شيء أخر المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
أثر التأثير الاسترجاعي على المناخ The Effects of Feedback on Climate
2024-11-24
عمليات الخدمة اللازمة للجزر
2024-11-24
العوامل الجوية المناسبة لزراعة الجزر
2024-11-24
الجزر Carrot (من الزراعة الى الحصاد)
2024-11-24
المناخ في مناطق أخرى
2024-11-24
أثر التبدل المناخي على الزراعة Climatic Effects on Agriculture
2024-11-24

الدوافع إلى التبني قديما
27-5-2022
Stick ’Em Up! Sticky Notes
31-1-2017
حسن بن جعفر بن خضر كاشف الغطاء.
20-7-2016
حلم أشجار الثويا الكاذب Thuja False Red Mite
8-7-2021
Dyson Mod 27 Identities
22-8-2019
Graphs: Traversing Roads
9-2-2016

Reflection: The discursive approach and relevance theory  
  
212   01:31 صباحاً   date: 25-5-2022
Author : Jonathan Culpeper and Michael Haugh
Book or Source : Pragmatics and the English Language
Page and Part : 216-7

Reflection: The discursive approach and relevance theory

Watts (e.g. 2003) embraces relevance theory (Sperber and Wilson [1986]1995) as an explanatory framework. Mills (2003) also argues, though not uncritically, that relevance theory can make a contribution to the discursive approach (see also Christie 2007: 278–279). Given that relevance theory is a “grand” theory of universal application, this would seem a rather odd move. It has been used to account for politeness by a number of scholars (e.g. Escandell-Vidal 1996; Jary 1998; Christie 2007). In particular, relevance theory can account for the anticipated versus inferred distinction. So few cognitive effects arise from anticipated politeness (behavior following social norms) that it is not relevant enough to spend inferential effort on it; but when there are sufficiently large cognitive effects to reward processing effort, inferred politeness can take place. What makes it attractive to scholars pursuing the discursive approach is that it emphasizes the hearer and does not have generalized norms of behavior as a starting point, instead, it focuses on specific situated behaviors, “it provides an extremely powerful interpretive apparatus” (Watts 2003: 212). However, relevance theory has three problems for politeness-related studies. First, the relevance theory account of communication still involves the recognition of speakers’ intentions. It does not suit politeness to place a relatively restricted notion of intention at its centre. Second, Haugh (2003: 406) points out that the notion of cognitive effects has not been sufficiently characterized:

[T]here is no distinction made between cognitive effects which have “positive effect” (such as feelings of approval or warmth and so on), and those which have “negative effect” (such as antagonism or alienation and so on). For example, there is no distinction made between showing that one thinks well of others (which can give rise to politeness), and showing that one thinks badly of others (which can give rise to impoliteness).

Third, no publication has shown how relevance theory can produce effective analyses of stretches of naturally occurring discourse, a limitation Watts (2003: 212) concedes: “[o]ne major problem with RT is that it rarely, if ever, concerns itself with stretches of natural verbal interaction”.