Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Semiotics
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Teaching Methods
Teaching Strategies
Foregrounding
المؤلف:
Jonathan Culpeper and Michael Haugh
المصدر:
Pragmatics and the English Language
الجزء والصفحة:
64-3
30-4-2022
812
Informational foreground
Foregrounding
In informational pragmatics, scholars have not fully explained what it is that pushes information into the foreground and makes it salient. Some work done on the pragmatic-grammar interface has addressed more formal aspects of foregrounding, and we will consider some of this work . Nevertheless, too many studies simply refer to, for example, something having been recently mentioned as evidence that it is in the foreground. There is much more to it than this. A key theory to turn to here is the theory of foregrounding. This theory does not restrict its focus to the sentence, but can work at various linguistic levels. It is concerned with how language can be made cognitively salient, with – recalling our distinction between semantic givenness/newness and cognitive givenness/newness – cognitive focus. Though rooted in Russian Formalism and the work of the Prague School, its main development came about in the 1960s and after in literary studies, discourse analysis and cognitive psychology. Foregrounding theory has been shown to guide the interpretation of literary texts (van Peer 1986), though there is no reason for limiting it to literary texts since it encapsulates general principles.
Foregrounded elements achieve salience through deviation from a linguistic norm (Mukarovský 1970). This idea is analogous to that of figure and ground. The characteristics of such foregrounded elements include “unexpectedness, unusualness, and uniqueness” (ibid.: 53–54). Unexpectedness is a notion that is not remote from cognitive newness as opposed to givenness . Consider an example of foregrounding. In Shakespeare’s Anthony and Cleopatra, when Anthony says, Let Rome in Tiber melt (I.i.33), deviation occurs at a semantic level, since, quite obviously, a city cannot melt. Our attention is captured and we can work to construct an interpretation. Leech (1985) drew attention to the two sides of foregrounding: just as you can have deviation through irregularity, so you can also have deviation through regularity. The parallelism of the same kinds of elements where you would normally expect different ones is a type of deviation. In the above example, one can perceive a high degree of regularity in the metrical pattern:
The strict alternation between stressed and unstressed syllables picks this utterance out. It is foregrounded not only against the natural rhythms of spoken English, but also against the immediately preceding lines which contain no such regularity of patterning. This regularity helps foreground the serious nature of what Anthony has to say, his first statement of treason. There is also some degree of syntactic foregrounding. Syntactic norms are violated in that no definite article precedes Tiber. Furthermore, the ordering of this particular clause, the adverbial falling between subject and verb, seems odd. Perhaps the syntactic construction here was motivated by a desire to form a regular metrical pattern.
One of the main reasons for the success of the notion of foregrounding lies in its relevance to the study of the process of linguistic interpretation. Leech (1985: 47) points out that more interpretive effort is focused on foregrounded elements in an attempt to rationalize their abnormality, than on backgrounded elements. Leech writes:
In addition to the normal processes of interpretation which apply to texts, whether literary or not, foregrounding invites an act of IMAGINATIVE INTERPRETATION by the reader. When an abnormality comes to our attention, we try to make sense of it. We use our imaginations, consciously or unconsciously, in order to work out why this abnormality exists. (Leech 1985: 47)
Furthermore, van Peer (1986) has provided empirical evidence to substantiate the claim that foregrounded elements are not only cognitively more striking but are also regarded as more important in relation to the overall interpretation of the text.
Foregrounding theory is generally focused on implicit foregrounding, achieved by making certain elements cognitively salient and thus likely to attract more attention. Sometimes, language deploys explicit foregrounding. An example of this is when we write in this topic “note that ...”. Here, we are explicitly directing you towards parts of the text which we think are important and so should be in the foreground of your attention (you can always ignore the instruction of course!). There are, needless to say, other structures that involve that kind of explicit direction and we will attend to some of them, amongst other things.
الاكثر قراءة في pragmatics
اخر الاخبار
اخبار العتبة العباسية المقدسة

الآخبار الصحية
