Read More
Date: 7-5-2017
2426
Date: 19-3-2016
2096
Date: 7-5-2017
3986
|
Personal Errors
Many measurements require personal judgments. Examples include estimating the position of a pointer between two scale divisions, the color of a solution at the end point in a titration, or the level of a liquid with respect to a graduation in a pipet or buret. Judgments of this type are often subject to systematic, unidirectional errors. For example, one person may read a pointer consistently high, while another may be slightly slow in activating a timer. Yet, a third may be less sensitive to color changes, with an analyst who is insensitive to color changes tending to use excess reagent in a volumetric analysis. Analytical procedures should always be adjusted so that any known physical limitations of the analyst cause negligibly small errors. Automation of analytical procedures can eliminate many errors of this type.
A universal source of personal error is prejudice, or bias. Most of us, no matter how honest, have a natural, subconscious tendency to estimate scale readings in a direction that improves the precision in a set of results. Alternatively, we may have a preconceived notion of the true value for the measurement. We then subconsciously cause the results to fall close to this value. Number bias is another source of personal error that varies considerably from person to person. The most frequent number bias encountered in estimating the position of a needle on a scale involves a preference for the digits 0 and 5. Also common is a prejudice favoring small digits over large and even numbers over odd. Again, automated and computerized instruments can eliminate this form of bias.
|
|
تفوقت في الاختبار على الجميع.. فاكهة "خارقة" في عالم التغذية
|
|
|
|
|
أمين عام أوبك: النفط الخام والغاز الطبيعي "هبة من الله"
|
|
|
|
|
المجمع العلمي ينظّم ندوة حوارية حول مفهوم العولمة الرقمية في بابل
|
|
|