Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Classifiers
المؤلف:
PETER SVENONIUS
المصدر:
Adjectives and Adverbs: Syntax, Semantics, and Discourse
الجزء والصفحة:
P21-C2
2025-03-27
77
Classifiers
Many languages have head-like elements in the DP which are called “classifiers” (cf. Aikhenvald 2000 for an overview). These show a range of uses, from determiner-like (cf. Cheng and Sybesma 1999 on Chinese) to being involved in enumeration, quantification, or division of masses (Borer 2005a) and other functions.1 Classifiers often serve two or more of these functions simultaneously and are often furthermore in complementary distribution with determiners or plural markers, making general statements about relative order difficult, but some generalizations emerge.
For example, the most typical classifier sorts nominal referents by characteristics such as shape (a “sortal” classifier), and makes them countable or quantifiable (a “numeral” classifier). Grinevald (2000) in particular argues that it is typical of numeral classifiers that they sort by shape. But a few languages differentiate the two functions, in which case the numeral classifier can be seen to be outside the sortal classifier, as in the Mayan language Akatek as described by Zavala (2000) (exx. here from his pp. 117 and 123).
Numeral classifiers in this language distinguish human, animal, and inanimate nouns (here only the inanimate one is shown). The sortal classifier distinguishes a dozen or more shapes (“smooth,” “long three-dimensional,” “erect,” half circle,” “round,” “wide flat,” “small spherical,” “separate,” etc.). Note in (7a–b) that the same noun can appear with different classifiers, depending on how the referent is perceived.
A third important type of classifier is the noun classifier, which typically sorts nouns by material qualities or essences (see Craig 1986, Grinevald 2000). These sometimes cooccur with sortal or numeral classifiers, and again, Akatek provides an example of cooccurrence. Akatek has a set of fourteen noun classifiers (“man, “woman,” “animal,” “tree,” “corn,” “water,” “salt,” etc.) alongside the three numeral classifiers and the set of sortal classifiers. All three types are illustrated in (8) (adapted from Zavala 2000: 126–127).2
Noun classifier systems may resemble gender systems, in that a noun may be conventionally associated with a single noun classifier, and may develop into gender systems (Greenberg 1978, Corbett 1991). It is sometimes argued that nominal gender corresponds to a functional projection (cf. e.g. Spanish abuelo ‘grandfather’ ∼ abuela ‘grandmother’ or monje ‘monk’ ∼ monja ‘nun’; cf. Ritter 1993 for references and a dissenting view). If so, it is clear that it is lower than number (cf. e.g. abuelas ‘grandmothers’ etc.).
In general, if a language has plural markers it does not have classifiers, and vice versa (see Borer 2005a: ch. 4 for references and discussion). In some languages, there are plural markers for animate nouns but classifiers for inanimates. In Akatek, the numeral classifier for inanimates, eb’ (cf. 8b–c), is also used as a separate plural marker for human plurals, and may cooccur not only with numeral classifiers but also with noun classifiers, as in (9a–b) (Zavala 2000: 122–123; again, I have isolated noun phrases from sentences in context; see Zavala’s paper for original sentences).
Nor is Akatek the only language in which a plural marker may cooccur with a numeral classifier. Allan (1977: 294) gives the following examples from Yucatec Mayan and Ojibway, respectively. In the Mayan example, the plural marker is optional, but in the Ojibway example, it has a semantic effect.
On the basis of these considerations, three levels of classifier can be identified; I will refer to the numeral classifier as unit, as this is the unit which is counted (it can be equated with Borer’s 2005a #), and to the sortal classifier as sort (like Borer’s Cl); and I will refer to the noun classifier as n, following the discussion in Marantz (2001) of the nature of nominalizing affixes on roots.
In sum, the following hierarchy of classifier types can be discerned, though it should be stressed that most classifier languages have just one which spans two or more of these properties, as argued by Borer (2005a):3
Putting these together with the Art > Pl > N hierarchy, it is relatively clear that Art is above these classifiers, while N is below. The exact position of Pl is partly a matter of guesswork due to the scarcity of clear cooccurrences, but the following seems to be a plausible hypothesis:
The category Pl/sort suggests that those cases in which plurals are noted to cooccur with classifiers have involved either unit classifiers or noun classifiers, not sort classifiers. It has repeatedly been argued that classifiers exist to individuate masses for quantification and counting (cf. in particular Borer 2005a).
At this juncture I leave these categories and turn to phrasal dependents in the DP, namely demonstratives, numerals, and adjectives.
1 On the distinction between classifiers and markers of noun class or gender like the nominal prefixes of the Bantu languages, cf. e.g. Heine (1982), Dixon (1986).
2 Zavala gives sentences from which I have isolated just the noun phrases; in each sentence, an existential predicate (“there is”) precedes the noun phrase, and an adjectival predicate (“lying down “or “round”) follows.
3 It should be noted that Grinevald (2000) argues for an additional type, the genitive classifier, which she argues is function-based (vehicle, edible, artifact, etc.) and is higher than the numeral classifier.