Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Rival morphological processes 4: Where have all the rivals gone?
المؤلف:
Ingo Plag
المصدر:
Morphological Productivity
الجزء والصفحة:
P227-C8
2025-02-13
290
Rival morphological processes 4:
Where have all the rivals gone?
The central question I would like to answer is which mechanisms regulate the selection of the appropriate affix with a given base. The solution of this problem has important consequences for certain theoretical issues such as the role of paradigmatic mechanisms in morphology or the modeling of morphological processes. It will turn out that the distribution of affixes is largely governed by the individual properties of the processes in question, complemented only by token-blocking and local analogy. This analysis supports a sign-based view of morphology and challenges separationist theories like Beard's (e.g. 1995).
In order to investigate their rivalry, the processes will be compared with each other, starting with -ize and -ify. Before we turn to this task, I will briefly define what is meant by the term 'rival'. In general, morphological processes are regarded as rival if they are phonologically distinct but semantically identical. This phenomenon is also often discussed under the label of affix synonymy, and in separationist approaches to morphology synonymy is one of the central arguments for these theories. On closer inspection, however, it turns out that many of the putatively rival processes are not really rivals in this sense.
For example, Baayen and Lieber (1991) claim that the English adjectival suffixes -ous and -ish are rivals, one attaching to Latinate bases, the other to Germanic bases. Malkiel (1977) demonstrates however that the two are not synonymous, -ish deriving a qualitative adjective indicating similarity, -ous being purely relational. The notorious couple -ity and -ness is a parallel case, since, as convincingly argued by Riddle (1985), these two suffixes exhibit subtle semantic differences. A similar point is made by Doyle (1992) with respect to Irish nominalizations. From these studies one can draw the conclusion that the domains in which two processes are actual rivals are often much smaller than standardly assumed. In some cases no overlap exists at all, making the assumption of rivalry an artefact of an insufficient analysis. The remaining overlap between domains may be further curtailed by additional restrictions, e.g. phonological ones, with the consequence that the number of cases where there is indeed a choice between affixes is further reduced.
What does this mean for the verb-deriving processes in English? As already mentioned, the different affixes are not completely synonymous but merely overlap in meaning to varying degrees. This finding leads to a considerable reduction in the number of potentially rival forms. As we will shortly see, the phonological restrictions on the suffixes further diminish the potentially overlapping domains. The remaining truly rival domains are often very small, so that the number of actually competing derivatives is drastically diminished. It will become clear that in the remaining phonologically and semantically overlapping domains no additional restrictions can be discerned. In other words, the overall distribution of the affixes is only restricted by the particular combinatorial characteristics of each individual affix, i.e. its semantic and phonological properties.